An Investigation on Turkish Specialist and Practitioner Doctors’ Empathy and Cognitive Flexibility Levels and Interpersonal Relationship Styles

By Harika Kökalan Yeşil, MS; Dr. Oguz Tan, PhD; Ayce Feride Yılmaz, MA, MS

Facebook
Twitter
LinkedIn

Citation

Yeşil H, Tan O, Yılmaz A. An investigation on Turkish specialist and practitioner doctors’ empathy and cognitive flexibility levels and interpersonal relationship styles. HPHR. 2021;28.

DOI:10.54111/0001/bb1

An Investigation on Turkish Specialist and Practitioner Doctors’ Empathy and Cognitive Flexibility Levels and Interpersonal Relationship Styles

Abstract

The aim of this research was to compare general practitioners and specialist doctors’ empathy and cognitive flexibility levels along with their interpersonal relationship styles. Research participants involved 30 general practitioners working in family health centers and 30 specialists serving in state hospitals. Data were obtained via sociodemographic information form, Jefferson Scale of Empathy, Cognitive Flexibility Inventory, and Interpersonal Relationship Scale. Although empathy levels did not differ between specialists and practitioners, findings revealed specialists embraced more open, respectful, and less condescending interpersonal relations styles than practitioners. Relatedly, specialists’ cognitive flexibility-alternatives subscale scores were significantly higher than practitioners. While practitioners’ cognitive flexibility-alternatives subscales were positively correlated with respectful styles and negatively correlated with condescending ones, their cognitive flexibility-control subscales were positively associated with respectful styles. Similarly, in this group, empathy was positively related to both cognitive flexibility subscales and negatively related to condescending styles. For specialists, empathy was not correlated with cognitive flexibility or interpersonal relations style. Demonstrating the importance and implications of communicational and relational interactions between patients and physicians seems to be an urgent issue for research. As a cost-effective measure, empathy and cognitive flexibility can be both learned and improved.

1. Introduction

The interlocked relationship between mind and body has increasingly resulted in modern medical practices’ use of bio-psycho-social and patient-centered models that emphasize various psychological and environmental factors (Larivaara et al., 2001, p. 8 ; Sobel, 2000, p. 393; Smith et al., 2002, p. 459).

 

In particular, patient-centered approaches are characterized by a serious consideration of patients’ views, preferences, values, and economic resources (Sacristán, 2013, p. 2). In addition to positively affecting various medical outcomes and patient satisfaction levels, patient-centered approaches have been shown to reduce the use of healthcare services, diagnostic tests, prescription, referral, and hospitalization, along with total annual healthcare expenses (Stewart et al., 2000, p. 796; Bertakis & Azari, 2011, p. 229).
Given that visiting a physician may in itself induce considerable worry and anxiety in patients (Bensing et al., 2008, p. 373), physicians’ interpersonal communication competencies and styles can directly influence patients’ self-expression by providing them with a sense of being listened to and understood, which in turn acts on several factors such as patients’ anxieties about their illness, hope for a cure, and adherence to the treatment (Bensing et al., 1996, p. 825). In the treatment of somatic diseases, talking about disease perception reveals the therapeutic effect of talking (van Dulmen & Bensing, 2002, p. 289).


In this regard, empathy refers to a “cognitive quality” that by its interpersonal nature involves a physician’s capacity to understand and communicate about patients’ experiences, anxieties, and viewpoints (Zaki et al., 2008, p. 399; Hojat, 2007). Empathy has been reported to reduce patients’ anxiety and stress levels and to considerably influence positive health outcomes (Derksen et al., 2013, p. 76). More specifically, physicians’ empathy has been shown to be significantly associated with diabetes patients’ clinical outcomes and result in shorter periods of the common cold (Hojat et al., 2011, p. 359; Del Canale et al., 2012, p. 1243; Rakel et al., 2009, p. 494; Rakel et al., 2011, p. 390). As these studies point to a concrete and measurable effect in the immune system induced by a positively experienced subjective feeling, they further substantiate the strong relationship between mind and body.

 

Among the factors that have transformed the physician-patient relationship from Emanuel’s paternalistic model to a conciliatory model are the rising socioeconomic level with social development, medical technologies that improve the quality of life, the development of patients’ rights, patients’ greater knowledge of their rights, developments in the concept of seeking rights, physicians and patients putting more value on human rights, the widespread implementation of legal responsibilities and sanctions on physicians and patients, legal obligations, the application and dissemination of the understanding of quality in healthcare, rising health literacy, and the existence of complaint mechanisms through communication centers of public institutions (Öngören, 2017, p. 57).

 

Studies supporting patients’ empathy expectations in Turkey (Özmen, 2006; Bozkaya, 2008; Güldal et al., 2012, p. 107; Yıldız, 2019; Ölmez, 2019, Arslan, 2019) as well as focusing on patient-physician communication show that empathy stands out as a very important element, sometimes alone and sometimes together with other variables (Atıcı, 2007, p. 91; Teke et al., 2010, p. 505; Kutlu et al., 2010, p. 127; Atilla et al., 2012, p. 23). There are many studies that evaluate empathy education and empathetic attitudes of students in the basic medical education process in medical schools and investigate how empathy can be developed. (Dereboy et al., 2005, p. 2651; Gönüllü & Dökmeci, 2007, p. 651; Kadıoğlu et al., 2011,p.1300; Igde & Sahin, 2017, p. 1177; Gönüllü et al., 2017, p. 651; Sağlam, 2019, p. 62; Akgün, 2019; Baş Mengi, 2020).

 

As another significant variable of this study, cognitive flexibility is regarded as a form of fluid intelligence marked by the skill of providing alternative solutions to different situations (Silver et al., 2004, p. 93). This construct is closely related to the neuro-psychological concepts of role- and perspective-taking, which entail numerous cognitive flexibility dimensions such as understanding others, selecting appropriate behavior, thinking about and generating different ideas, possessing a repertoire for different responses, and exchanging ideas with others in decision-making (Eslinger, 1998, p. 193).

 

Literature on cognitive flexibility related to personnel working in the healthcare field is quite limited. A study on nurses found that coping skills and flexibility were positively correlated with psychological adjustment (Kruczek et al., 2020, p. 507). A study of medical students and residents showed that incorporating cognitive flexibility and perspective-taking skill instruction has implications for reducing conflict and stress, as well as improving wellness levels (Houser et al., 2018, p. 1). Empirical studies demonstrate that increased cognitive flexibility is linked to reduced levels of experiencing social difficulties, stress, depression, anxiety, and rage (Altunkol, 2011; Delano-Wood, 2002; Diril, 2011; Maltby et al., 2004, p. 1475; Öz, 2012). Conversely, cognitive flexibility has been found to be related to positive personal outcomes such as critical thinking, self-esteem, social skills, self-competence, and coping with stress (Al-Jabari & Rawya 2012; Çuhadaroğlu, 2011; Laçin and Yalçın, 2019; Öz, 2012).

 

Although limited amount of evidence exists in the literature, there are several empirical studies that substantiate the relationship between empathy and cognitive flexibility. In one major example, neurological patients with various etiologies and cerebral lesions were found to manifest significantly lower empathy than healthy normal adults. The same study found significant correlations, ranging from 0.5 to 0.6, between cognitive flexibility and empathy scores (Grattan & Eslinger, 1989, p. 175). These data support the idea that cognitive thinking may be closely linked to empathic behavior either via granting a precognitive skill or by being part of another common basic process.
The present study is designed to make comparisons between Turkish practitioner and specialist physicians in terms of empathy, cognitive flexibility, and interpersonal relations styles. In so doing, it tests the question of whether practitioners’ long-term interactions with their enrolled patients (i.e. spending more time with them, knowing them better) or specialists’ longer training histories are correlated with increased empathy and cognitive flexibility levels.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

It is included family physicians, who have completed 6 years of medical training and specialists who have completed an additional 5 years. Family physicians see the patients registered with them and specialists see patients who can make an appointment anytime and anywhere. Inclusion criteria for data collection were determined as for specialists who work as clinician. Branches of specialists: 5 orthopedics, 8 chest diseases, 1 neurology, 4 obstetrics, 3 infectious diseases, 2 internal medicine, 1 physical therapy, 1 pediatric, 3 ENT, 2 urology. General practitioner work as clinician. Thus, the sample of the present study comprises 60 participants, i.e. 30 practitioners and 30 specialists. Demographically, 40% of the practitioners and 47% of the specialists were women. Additionally, 90% of the practitioners and 97% of the specialists were married. The age average for practitioners and specialists was 45.5 and 43.2, respectively. As mentioned above, participants’ working posts involved family health centers and state or training and research hospitals. Paper-and-pencil questionnaires were filled out by volunteer participants themselves in the major cities of Istanbul and Ankara between January and April 2019. The questions that the study aims to measure in terms of the variables included are as follows: Do long-term interaction and being familiar affect physicians’ interpersonal relationship styles? Does residency training affect specialist’ relationship styles?

2.2 Assessment Instruments

To address its research questions, this research employed the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, the Cognitive Flexibility Inventor, the Interpersonal Relationship Scale, and a socio-demographic form.

2.2.1. Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy

The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSPE) is a 20-item, 7-point Likert-type scale. In a sample of medical doctors, its internal consistency was found to be around .80. In the Turkish context, this scale was adapted by Malkondu in 2006 and its validity-reliability study was done on a sample of dentists (Gönüllü & Dökmeci, 2013, p. 53).

2.2.2. Cognitive Flexibility Inventory

Developed by Dennis and Vander, the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory involves 20 questions and two subscales entitled “alternatives” and “control” (Gülüm & Dağ, 2012, p. 216). The alternatives subscale comprises 13 items that tap into the extent to which an individual is able to find alternative solutions to difficult conditions or can form alternative explanations for life situations and people’s behaviors. The control subscale is constituted by items that measure to what extent these predicaments can be controlled (Sapmaz & Doğan, 2013, p. 143). The Cronbach’s alpha values for alternatives and control subscale were reported as .91 and .84, respectively. Higher scores are indicative of increased cognitive flexibility (Gülüm & Dağ, 2012, p.216). Validity and reliability research for the scale’s Turkish version was undertaken by Gülüm and Dağ (2012), who found Cronbach’s alpha values of .90 for the whole scale, .89 for the alternatives subscale, and .85 for the control subscale.

2.2.3. Interpersonal Relationship Scale

Developed by Şahin, Durak, and Yasak (1994), this 31-item, 3-point Likert-type scale assesses individuals’ interaction styles with others. Two subscales measure nurturing and restraining styles, respectively. Nurturing relations involve skills such as expressing one’s needs in an open manner, treating the other person with a respectful and accepting style, and employing a constructivist discourse. Conversely, restraining communication styles are marked by behaviors such as self-righteousness, condescension, short temper, verbal abuse, and mockery. The scale has been used in many studies and evidence has been obtained that it is valid and reliable (Şahin et al., 1994, p. 17; Oktay& Batıgün 2014, p. 41; Akdur & Batıgün 2017, p. 11; Hasta& Güler 2013, p. 64) Subscales for nurturing styles, in turn, tap into open and respective styles, while the restraining styles subscale further comprises egocentric and condescending styles. The internal consistency coefficient for the whole scale was measured as .79. Cronbach’s alpha values for open, respectful, egocentric, and condescending styles were .73, .70, .56, and .78, respectively.

3. Results

As reassessed from the data, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the empathy scale turned out to be .847, while the cognitive flexibility subscales of control and alternatives had internal consistency values of .653 and .749, respectively. The interpersonal relationship subscales’ alpha coefficients ranged from 0.653 to 0.749.

 

In terms of between-group comparisons, Mann-Whitney U tests indicated that, although specialists’ empathy level was higher than practitioners, the difference was not statistically significant. With respect to cognitive flexibility comparisons, Mann-Whitney U and t-tests substantiated that only the cognitive flexibility-alternatives subscale was significantly higher in specialists than practitioners. The associated values for these comparisons are shown in Table 1 and Table 2.

 

Interpersonal relationship style comparisons centered on nurturing (open-respectful) and restraining (ego centric-condescending) styles. As scores for open and egocentric styles were normally distributed for both groups, t-tests were employed for comparisons. Conversely, distributions for respectful and condescending styles were not normally distributed. Thus, the Mann-Whitney U test was preferred for comparisons in these domains. Accordingly, results indicated that specialists received significantly higher scores than practitioners in nurturing styles and significantly lower scores in restraining styles (Table 3).

 

Spearman-Brown correlational analyses revealed a significant positive association between empathy and cognitive flexibility for practitioners. Similarly, for this group, the relationship between empathy and respectful relationship styles was also significantly positive, while the correlation between empathy and condescending styles was significantly negative (Table 4). Specialists’ correlations between empathy and cognitive flexibility and relationship styles were not significant (Table 5). Regarding correlations between cognitive flexibility and relationship styles, for practitioners, control and alternatives subscales were moderately associated with respectful styles. For specialists, cognitive and alternatives subscales were significantly related to open styles. For this group, an alternatives subscale was also moderately related to respectful styles. These significant positive associations are shown in Tables 6 and 7, respectively.

 

Gender and marital status were not significantly related to specialists’ or practitioners’ empathy tendencies, relationship styles, cognitive flexibility total, or subscale scores. For practitioners, having longer careers contributed significantly to increased empathy, cognitive flexibility levels, and nurturing styles. For this group, age and cognitive flexibility was also positively related. For specialists, age and working duration were not significantly correlated with empathy levels, relationship styles, or cognitive flexibility levels. These results are shown in Table 8.

Table 1. Comparison of empathy levels between specialists and practitioners

Group

N

Average

Std. Deviation

U

P

Level of empathy

Practitioner

30

97.63

17.84

367.500

.222

Specialist

30

101.30

15.86

According to the Mann-Whitney U test, although specialists’ empathy levels were higher than practitioners’, this difference was not statistically significant, (p>0.05).

Table 2. Comparison of cognitive flexibility levels between specialists and practitioners.

Group

N

Average

Std. Deviation

U / t

P

Control subscale

Practitioners

30

3.69

.82

U=318.000

.050

Specialists

30

4.10

.66

Alternatives

Practitioners

30

3.79

.52

t= -2.737

.008*

Specialists

30

4.14

.460

According to the Mann-Whitney U and t-tests, alternatives subscale was the only statistically significant difference, and scores were higher for specialists, (p<0.05).

Table 3. Comparison of interpersonal relationship styles between practitioners and specialists

Group

N

Average

Std. Deviation

U / t

P

Open

Practitioners

30

1.73

.37

t= -2.139

.037*

Specialists

30

1.95

.42

Respectful

Practitioners

30

1.54

.51

U=257.500

.004*

Specialists

30

1.91

.39

Egocentric

Practitioners

30

.88

.43

t= 1.248

.217

Specialists

30

.74

.44

Condescending

Practitioners

30

.49

.32

U= 312.500

.039*

Specialists

30

.31

.19

According to Mann-Whitney U and t-tests, significant differences were found in terms of open, respectful, and condescending styles between practitioners and specialists. Specifically, specialists displayed higher open and respectful style scores and lower condescending style scores. (*p<0.05)

Table 4. Correlations between empathy and cognitive flexibility and empathy and interpersonal relationship styles in practitioners.

 

Cognitive flexibility in Control Domain

Cognitive Flexibility in Alternatives Domain

Open

Respectful

Egocentric

Condescending

Level of empathy

r

.686

.631

.261

.506

-.358

-.514

p

.000*

.000*

.164

.004*

.052

.004*

According to the Spearman-Brown analysis, positive significant correlations were found between empathy and cognitive flexibility control and alternatives subscales. Empathy was also positively related to respectful styles. The sizes of these relations were moderate. Another moderate correlation was found between empathy and condescending styles that was negative in nature.(*p<0.05)

Table 5. Correlations between empathy and cognitive flexibility and empathy and interpersonal relationship styles in specialists.

 

Cognitive flexibility in Control Domain

Cognitive Flexibility in Alternatives Domain

 

Open

 

Respectful

 

 

Egocentric

 

Condescending

Level of empathy

r

-.060

.267

.164

.244

-.161

-.076

p

.752

.154

.386

.193

.396

.689

Table 6. Correlation between cognitive flexibility and interpersonal relationship style in practitioners.

 

Open

Respectful

Egocentric

Condescending

Cognitive flexibility-control

 r

.327

.414

-.266

-.273

 p

.078

.023*

.156

.145

Cognitive flexibility-alternatives

 r

.333

.433

-.170

-.599

 p

.072

.017*

.370

.000*

According to the Spearman-Brown analysis, significant moderate associations were found between cognitive flexibility alternatives and respectful and condescending styles that were positive and negative in nature, respectively. There was also a positive moderate correlation between cognitive flexibility control and respectful style. (*p<0.05)

Table 7. Correlation between cognitive flexibility and interpersonal relationship style in specialists

 

Open

Respectful

Egocentric

Condescending

Cognitive flexibility-control

r

.382

.313

.115

-.117

p

.037*

.092

.546

.539

Cognitive flexibility-alternatives

r

.580

.633

-.112

-.331

p

.001*

.000*

.555

.074

According to Spearman-Brown correlation, the cognitive flexibility alternatives subscale was positively and moderately related to open and respectful styles. The cognitive flexibility-control subscale was positively correlated to open style. (*p<0.05)

Table 8. Associations between Empathy/Cognitive Flexibility and Age/Working Duration

      Practitioner Specialist
Age Working duration Age Working duration
Level of empathy r .375 .517* .090 .059
p .059 .008 .699 .759
Cognitive Flexibility-Control r .515* .644* .056 – .048
p .007 .001 .810 .802
Cognitive Flexibility-Alternatives r .337 .363 .056 .042
p .093 .074 .810 .826
Open r .193 .353 -.268 -.134
p .344 .084 .240 .480
Respectful r .330 .506* -.204 -.120
p .099 .010 .375 .528
Egocentric r -.072 -.083 .342 .176
p .725 .695 .129 .352
Condescending r -.321 -.217 .112 .075
p .110 .297 .629 .694
*p<0.05

4. Discussion and conclusion

4.1. Discussion

Findings revealed specialists’ empathy and cognitive flexibility scores were generally higher than practitioners’, yet the only statistically significant dimension was cognitive flexibility-alternatives in specialists.

 

In interpersonal relations, significant differences in terms of open, respectful, and condescending communication styles demonstrated that, compared to practitioners, specialist physicians were again more competent in these areas.

 

These findings do not corroborate our tentative assumption when undertaking the study, namely that due to familiarity and longer interactions with their patients, practitioners should exhibit higher empathy, cognitive flexibility, and communication competence. Our particular pattern may have stemmed from various factors: First, with respect to cognitive flexibility, due to certain preexisting cognitive skills, individuals who become specialists may be predisposed to more competently assess alternatives in particular situations and problems. Indeed, Spiro and Jehng (1990) proposed that the predominant characteristic of experts is the ability to manipulate versatile mental representations, which in turn enables them to adapt better to environmental changes and use their knowledge more efficiently among different tasks. Alternatively, one may consider the effect of specialists’ education in terms of both different cognitive skills acquired and being exposed to courses on patient-doctor interactions. It is conceivable that different specialist branches, the cultures of hospitals and medical schools, socioeconomic status, or even cities of residence may play a role in augmenting or hindering cognitive flexibility, empathy, and interpersonal relationship style. In this regard, future studies may include these variables to determine both their unique and interactive effects for various professionals in the medical field.

 

Although not displaying a statistically significant difference, higher empathy levels and interpersonal relations competence of specialists may be linked to particular predicaments of the medical system in Turkey. In Turkey, practitioners regularly receive a burdensome number of registered patients. In this sense, it is reasonable to suggest that practitioners may have developed certain emotive strategies to mitigate the emotional burden that accompanies personal interactions with their patients. Alternatively, the mundane and repetitive tasks of referrals to specialists and renewing prescriptions may also hamper the tendency to develop a genuinely empathetic understanding of patients’ problems and emotional states.

 

Conversely, specialists in Turkey accept additional patients in return for extra income, which offers them an opportunity to control the overburden they experience. These points highlight the importance of considering the significant role that the nature of healthcare systems may play in determining the quality of interpersonal communication and empathetic interaction. It is particularly telling that both specialist and practitioner empathy scores in Turkey are well below those reported by international studies, which lends support to the idea that predicaments in healthcare systems may greatly compromise cognitive flexibility and empathy. As compared to our scores of 101.30 for specialists and 97.63 for practitioners, Kataoka et al. reported international physicians’ empathy levels in human-centered and technology-centered specialty fields as 112.9 and 106.9, respectively. Average physician empathy levels were found to be 120 in the United States (Hojat et al., 2001, p. 349), 115.1 in Italy (Di Lillo, 2009, p. 1198), 122 in Spain (Alcorta-Garza et al., 2016, p. 1), 98.2 in South Korea (Suh et al., 2012, p. 464), 110 in Iran (Shariat et al., 2010, p. 417), 113.06 in Poland (Kliszcz et al., 2006, p. 219), and 102.85 in the Czech Republic (Kožený & Tišanská, 2013, p. 521).

 

In another major finding, in a practitioner sample within group analyses, the present study substantiated empathy’s significant relation to cognitive flexibility and interpersonal relations. These findings resonate with a prior study on Turkish university students (Hasta & Güler, 2013, p. 64) in which, as in this study too, empathy was found to be positively associated with open and respectful styles and negatively linked to condescending styles. Since in our study, empathy in specialists was not correlated to cognitive flexibility or communication style, one may consider the idea that, for specialists, the more intermittent and impersonal nature of doctor-patient interactions in the Turkish healthcare system enables the acquisition of satisfactory communication skills mainly via cognitive skills. Conversely, for health professionals such as practitioners and nurses who interact more closely with patients, communication skills may be induced mainly along the path of empathy.

 

Notwithstanding fields or branches among health professionals, the present study can be considered a noteworthy contribution to international research that investigates questions regarding empathy, cognitive skills, communication, and patient outcomes for healthcare professionals and workers at large. Indeed, a recent upsurge in international studies started to increasingly point to relations of these kinds. To give a few examples, prior research found a positive relationship between empathy and interpersonal relations in nursing students (Chung, 2014, p. 332; Kim, 2018, p. 229), and similarly posited positive relationships among empathy, perspective-taking, nurturing care, and friendly-harmonious relationship types in medical students (Jordan & Foster, 2016, p. 1009). Recently a strong relationship was suggested between empathic thinking and perspective-taking (Song & Shi, 2017, p. 1). Hence, the present study lends support to the idea that the empathic and communicational skills of medical professionals should be one of the major avenues of research for promoting patient outcomes.

 

This study offers ideas as to why training intervention is needed to improve communication and interpersonal relationships, and also includes suggestions about what the training program should cover.

 

Some limitations should also be noted. The rather small sample sizes, including 30 participants for each group, and non-random, voluntary sampling procedures in this study should be considered caveats preventing satisfactory representativeness and generalizations around the globe. Additionally, variables such as doctors’ and patients’ personality traits and cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds were excluded from analyses. Lastly, our measuring instruments relied on self-report scales and are thus subject to biases rooted in the conveyance of subjective experience. These points should be taken into account by researchers while drawing cautious inferences for future research studies.

4.2. Conclusion

All in all, this study provides support to bio-psycho-social model patient-centered approaches and suggests their adoption positively influences patients’ psychological, mental, and physical health. Hence, the cultivation of an understanding recognizing the importance of interpersonal relations, communication skills, and interaction between the mind and body seems to be a worthwhile endeavor in the medical field.

 

The long-term articulation of cost-effective methods in healthcare systems – primarily training in factors promoting interpersonal relationships such as empathy and cognitive flexibility – would positively contribute to more efficient processes of diagnosis and therapy based on fulfilling interactions between physicians and patients.

References

  1. Akdur, S., & Batıgün Durak, A. (2017). The Relationships Between Humor Styles, Personality Styles, Interpersonal Relationship Style and Psychological Health. Turkish Psychological Articles, 20(39), 11-13 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ayseguel-Batiguen-2/publication/319122667
  2. Akgün, Ö. (2019). Comparison of general empathy levels and medical empathy skills of Akdeniz University Faculty of Medicine students and evaluation of the change in empathy levels during medical education. [Master’s Thesis, University of Akdeniz]
  3. Alcorta-Garza A., San-Martín M., Delgado-Bolton R., Soler-González J., Roig H., & Vivanco L. (2016). Cross-Validation of the Spanish HP-Version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy Confirmed with Some Cross-Cultural Differences. Frontiers Psychology, 7:1002. https://doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01002
  4. Al-Jabari A.B., & Rawya M. (2012). Relationships among self-esteem, psychological and cognitive flexibility, and psychological symptomatology [Master’s thesis, University of North Texas] https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc177172/
  5. Altunkol, F. (2011). The analysis of the relation between cognitive flexibility and perceived stress levels of college students [Master’s thesis, University of Cukurova]. toad.halileksi.net/sites/default/files/pdf/bilişsel-esneklik-olcegi-toad.pdf
  6. Arslan, D. (2019). Empathy in physicians with a patient’s overview and physicians’ exposure to violence. [Master’s Thesis, University of Health Sciences]
  7. Atıcı, E. (2007). Factors Affecting the Patient-Physician Relationship, Journal of Uludag University Faculty of Medicine, 33 (2) 91-96. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/420633
  8. Atilla G., Oksay A., & Erdem R. (2012). A qualitative preliminary study on physician-patient communication. Journal of Istanbul University Faculty of Communication, 2(43), 23 -37 https://dergipark.org.tr/en/download/article-file/213076
  9. Baş Mengi, F. Ö. (2020). Comparison of the general empathy levels and medical empathy skills of medical expertise students of akdeniz university medical faculty. [Master’s Thesis, University of Akdeniz]
  10. Bensing, J., Schreurs, K., & De Rijk, A. (1996). The role of general practitioner’s affective behaviour in medical encounters. Psychology and Health, 11, 825–838. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870449608400278
  11. Bensing, J.M., Verheul, W., & van Dulmen, A.M. (2008). Patient anxiety in the medical encounter: a study of verbal and nonverbal communication in general practice. Health Education, 108(5), 373- 383 https://doi.org/10.1108/09654280810899993
  12. Bertakis, K. D., & Azari, R. (2011) Patient-centered care is associated with decreased health care utilization. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 24(3), 229- 239. https://doi.org/10.3122/jabfm.2011.03.100170
  13. Bozkaya Oklay, A. (2008). Patient expectations and the results of the patient-physician interview in terms of these expectations [Master’s thesis, University of Adnan Menderes]
  14. Chung, M. S. (2014). Relations on Self-esteem, Empathy and Interpersonal Relationship for Reinforcing Competence in Communication of Nursing Students. The Journal of Korean Academic Society of Nursing Education, 20, 332-340. https://doi.org/10.5977/jkasne.2014.20.2.332
  15. Çuhadaroğlu, A. (2016). Predictors of Cognitive Flexibility, Cumhuriyet International Journal of Education, 2, 86-101, ISSN: 2147-1606
  16. Delano-Wood, L. M. (2002). The relationship between cognitive flexibility, depression, and anxiety in older adults (Publication Number: 1410678) [Master’s thesis, Michigan State University]
  17. Del Canale, S., Louis, D. Z., Maio, V., Wang, X., Rossi, G., Hojat, M., & Gonnella, J. S. (2012). The relationship between physician empathy and disease complications: an empirical study of primary care physicians and their diabetic patients in Parma, Italy. Academic medicine: journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges87(9), 1243–1249. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0b013e3182628fbf
  18. Dereboy, Ç., Harlak, H., Gürel, S., Gemalmaz, A., & Eskin, M. (2005). Teaching empathy in medical education. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry,16(2), 83-89. ISSN: 1300-2163 / 2651-3463
  19. Derksen, F., Bensing, J., & Lagro-Janssen, A. (2013). Effectiveness of empathy in general practice: a systematic review. British Journal of General Practice, 63(606)76-84. https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp13X660814
  20. Di Lillo, M., Cicchetti, A., Lo Scalzo, A., Taroni, F., & Hojat, M. (2009). The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: preliminary psychometrics and group comparisons in Italian physicians. Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges84(9), 1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0b013e3181b17b3f
  21. Diril, A. (2011). An investigation of cognitive flexibility in terms of sociodemographic variables and anger level and anger styles in adolescent [Master’s thesis, Cukurova University] http://libratez.cu.edu.tr/tezler/8482.pdf
  22. Doğan Laçin, B. G., & Yalçın, İ. (2019). Predictive roles of self-efficacy and coping strategies in cognitive flexibility among university students. Hacettepe University Journal of Education, 34(2), 358- 371. https://doi.org/10.16986/HUJE.2018037424
  23. Durak Batıgün, A., Hisli Şahin, N., Karslı Demirel, E. (2011). Stress, self-perception and interpersonal style in patients with physical illnesses. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry22, 245–254, PMID: 22143950
  24. Eslinger P. J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of empathy. European neurology39(4), 193–199. https://doi.org/10.1159/000007933
  25. Gönüllü, İ., & Dökmeci, F. (2013). Empathy Scores of “Ankara University School of Medicine” Fifth Year Students in Two Different Curricula; A Preliminary Report, Journal of Ankara University Faculty Medicine., 66, 53-58, https://doi.org/10.1501/Tıpfak_000000841
  26. Gönüllü, İ., Erden, Ş., & Gökmen, D. (2017). Evaluation of Student Empathy Level During the First Five Years of Medical Education at Ankara University. Journal of Higher Education and Science, 7(3), 651-656 https://doi.org/10.5961/jhes.2017.241
  27. Grattan, L.M., & Eslinger, P. J. (1989). Higher cognition and social behavior: Changes in cognitive flexibility and empathy after cerebral lesions. Neuropsychology, 3(3) 175–185. Htts://doi.org/10.1037/h0091764
  28. Güldal, A. D., Günvar, T., Mevsim, V., Kandemir Kuruoğlu, E., & Yıldırım, E. (2012). Do family medicine core competencies align with patients’ expectations?. Turkish Journal of Family Medicine,16(3), 107-112 ISSN: 1303-6637 / 1308-531X
  29. Gülüm, İ.V., & Dağ İ. (2012). The Turkish adaptation, validity and reliability study of the repetitive thinking questionnaire and the cognitive flexibility inventory, Anatolian. Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 216- 223. http://www.ihsandag.gen.tr/index_dosyalar/GulumveDag2012.pdf
  30. Hasta, D., & Güler, E. (2013). Aggression: an investigation in terms of interpersonal styles and empathy, Ankara University Journal of Social Sciences 4 (1), 64-104. https://doi.org/10.1501/sbeder_0000000051
  31. Hojat, M. et al., 2001 “The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: development and preliminary psychometric data” Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61: pp. 349-365.
  32. Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Nasca, T. J., Cohen, M. J. M., Gonnella, J. S., Erdmann, J. B., Veloski, J., & Magee, M. (2001). The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: Development and Preliminary Psychometric Data. Educational and Psychological Measurement61(2), 349–365. https://doi.org/10.1177/00131640121971158
  33. Hojat, M. (2007). Empathy in patient care: Antecedents, Development, Measurement, and Outcomes. Springer-Verlag New York. Inc
  34. Hojat, M., Louis, D. Z., Markham, F. W., Wender, R., Rabinowitz, C., & Gonnella, J. S. (2011). Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Academic medicine, 86(3), 359–364. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0b013e3182086fe1
  35. Houser, M. M., Worzella, G., Burchsted, S., Marquez, C., Domack, T., & Acevedo, Y. (2018). Wellness Skills for Medical Learners and Teachers: Perspective Taking and Cognitive Flexibility. MedEdPORTAL : the journal of teaching and learning resources14, 10674. https://doi.org/10.15766/mep_2374-8265.10674
  36. Igde, F. A., & Sahin, M. K. (2017). Changes in Empathy during Medical Education: An Example from Turkey. Pakistan journal of medical sciences, 33(5), 1177–1181. https://doi.org/10.12669/pjms.335.13074
  37. Jordan, K. D., & Foster, P. S. (2016). Medical student empathy: Interpersonal distinctions and correlates. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 21(5), 1009 –1022. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-016-9675-8
  38. Kadıoğlu, N. S., Ögenler, O., Kadıoğlu, F. G., & Okuyaz, S. (2011). The effect of clinical education on the empathy level of medical school students. İnönü University Faculty of Medicine Journal, 18 (1), 33-37. ISSN: 1300-1744
  39. Kataoka, H. U., Koide, N., Hojat, M., & Gonnella, J. S. (2012). Measurement and correlates of empathy among female Japanese physicians. BMC medical education12, 48. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-12-48
  40. Kim J. (2018). Factors influencing nursing students’ empathy. Korean journal of medical education30(3), 229–236. https://doi.org/10.3946/kjme.2018.97
  41. Kliszcz, J., Nowicka-Sauer, K., Trzeciak, B., Nowak, P., & Sadowska, A. (2006). Empathy in health care providers–validation study of the Polish version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy. Advances in Medical Sciences, 51, 219–225. PMID: 17357313
  42. Kožený, J., & Tišanská, L. (2013). The structure of the Jefferson scale of physician empathy in Czech physicians. ročník LVII / číslo 6, Československá psychologie, 51, 521-532.
  43. Kruczek, A., Basińska, M. A., & Janicka, M. (2020). Cognitive flexibility and flexibility in coping in nurses – the moderating role of age, seniority and the sense of stress. International journal of occupational medicine and environmental health33(4), 507–521. https://doi.org/10.13075/ijomeh.1896.01567
  44. Kutlu, M., Çolakoğlu, N., Özgüvenç Pay, Z. (2010). A research on the importance of empathy in the patient-physician relationship. Journal of Performance and Quality in Health, 127-143 https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/303558
  45. Larivaara, P., Kiuttu, J., & Taanila, A. (2001). The patient-centred interview: the key to biopsychosocial diagnosis and treatment. Scandinavian journal of primary health care19(1), 8–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/02813430130003452
  46. Maltby, J., Day, L., McCutcheon, L. E., Martin, M. M., & Cayanus, J. L. (2004). Celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility, and social complexity. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(7), 1475-1482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.004
  47. Oktay, B., & Batıgün Durak, A. (2014) Alexithymia: Attachment, Self Perception, Interpersonal Relationship Style and Anger, Turkish Psychological Articles, 17(33), 41-44https://www.psikolog.org.tr/tr/yayinlar/dergiler/1031828/tpy1301996120140000m000081.pdf
  48. Ölmez, E H. (2019). Measurement of patient’s perception levels on physician empathy and physcian empathy: example of private hospitals [Master’s thesis, Beykent University] https://tez.yok.gov.tr/UlusalTezMerkezi/tezSorguSonucYeni.jsp
  49. Öngören, B. (2017). Physician Patient Relations and Communication in Health Services. Journal of Medical Social Work, 57-69. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/322316807
  50. Öz, S., (2012). Analysis of the relationship between the cognitive flexibility, adaptation and anxiety levels in adolescents based on sex, socio-economic status and education level, [Master’s Thesis, University of Cukurova ] http://libratez.cu.edu.tr/tezler/8752.pdf
  51. Özmen, A. (2006). Expectations of pregnant women who applied to Zübeyde Hanım Maternity Hospital from health personnel in terms of empathic communication [Master’s Term Project, Ankara University] https://dspace.ankara.edu.tr/xmlui/bitstream/handle/20.500.12575/31848/tez.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
  52. Rakel, D. P., Hoeft, T. J., Barrett, B. P., Chewning, B. A., Craig, B. M., & Niu, M. (2009). Practitioner empathy and the duration of the common cold. Family medicine41(7), 494–501.
  53. Rakel, D., Barrett, B., Zhang, Z., Hoeft, T., Chewning, B., Marchand, L., & Scheder, J. (2011). Perception of empathy in the therapeutic encounter: effects on the common cold. Patient education and counseling85(3), 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2011.01.009
  54. Sacristán J. A. (2013). Patient-centered medicine and patient-oriented research: improving health outcomes for individual patients. BMC medical informatics and decision making13(6), 2-8  https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-13-6
  55. Sağlam, E., (2019). An Overview of Empathy Education in Medical Schools, What Can Be Done to Improve?. World of Medical Education, 18(55), 62-69 https://doi.org/10.25282/ted.517847
  56. Sapmaz, F., & Doğan, T. (2013). Assessment of cognitive flexibility: Reliability and validity studies of Turkish version of the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory. Ankara University Journal of Faculty of Educational Sciences, 46 (1),143-162. https://doi.org/10.1501/Egifak_0000001278
  57. Shariat, S. V., Eshtad, E., & Ansari, S. (2010). Empathy and its correlates in Iranian physicians: A preliminary psychometric study of the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy. Medical teacher32(10), e417–e421. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2010.498488
  58. Smith, T. W., Kendall, P. C., & Keefe, F. J. (2002). Behavioral medicine and clinical health psychology: introduction to the special issue, a view from the decade of behavior. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology70(3), 459–462. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.70.3.459
  59. Sobel, D. S.(2000). The cost-effectiveness of mind-body medicine interventions. Progress in brain research122, 393–412. PMID: 10737073
  60. Song, Y., & Shi M. (2017). Associations between empathy and big five personality traits among Chinese undergraduate medical students. PLoS ONE 12(2). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171665
  61. Spiro, R. J., & Jehng, J.-C. (1990). Cognitive flexibility and hypertext: Theory and technology for the nonlinear and multidimensional traversal of complex subject matter. In D. Nix & R. J. Spiro (Eds.), Cognition, education, and multimedia: Exploring ideas in high technology (p. 163–205). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  62. Stewart, M., Brown, J. B., Donner, A., McWhinney, I. R., Oates, J., Weston, W. W., & Jordan, J. (2000). The impact of patient-centered care on outcomes. The Journal of family practice49(9), 796–804.
  63. Suh, D. H., Hong, J. S., Lee, D. H., Gonnella, J. S., & Hojat, M. (2012). The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: a preliminary psychometric study and group comparisons in Korean physicians. Medical teacher34(6), e464–e468. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2012.668632
  64. Silver, J. A., Hughes, J. D., Bornstein, R. A., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2004). Effect of anxiolytics on cognitive flexibility in problem solving. Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology17(2), 93–97. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnn.0000119240.65522.d9
  65. Şahin, N., Durak, A., & Yasak, Y. (1994, July, 17-22). Interpersonal style, loneliness and depression. [Conference presentation abstract]. Twenty third International Congress of Applied Psychology, Madrid, Spain
  66. Teke, K., Cengiz, E., & Demir, C. (2010). Measurement of physician’s empathic features and this change of measurements according to demographic variables. Çukurova University, Journal of the Social Sciences Institute, 19 (3), 505-516. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/50646
  67. van Dulmen, A. M., & Bensing, J. M. (2002). Health promoting effects of the physician-patient encounter. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 7(3), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548500220139421
  68. Yıldız, M. (2019). Evaluate the relationship between empathy measured in physicians and empathy as perceived by patients in these physicians. [Master’s Thesis, University of Health Sciences]
  69. Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. (2008). It takes two: the interpersonal nature of empathic accuracy. Psychological science19(4), 399–404. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008. 02099.x

About the Authors

Harika Kökalan Yeşil, MS

Harika Kökalan YeşilMS, Clinical Psychologist, works at the General Directorate of Health Promotion at the Ministry of Health of Turkey. Her focus area is communication in health care, interpersonal relations between physician and patient and patient-centered approach in the health care system. This study was conducted during her master degree study at University of Üsküdar.

Dr. Oguz Tan, MD

Dr. Oguz Tan, MD, is an Associate Professor of Psychiatry at the Neuropsychiatry Research and Practice Center, University of Üsküdar.

Ayce Feride Yılmaz, MA, MS

Ayce Feride Yılmaz, MA, MS, holds degrees in psychology, sociology and history. She is a PhD candidate at Yıldız Technical University, in the Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, in psychology and has an interest in narrative studies, critical psychology and gender.